Over-Attribution Bias Research Paper

This sample Over-Attribution Bias Research Paper is published for educational and informational purposes only. If you need help writing your assignment, please use our research paper writing service and buy a paper on any topic at affordable price. Also check our tips on how to write a research paper, see the lists of research paper topics, and browse research paper examples.

The over-attribution bias, also known as “correspondence bias,” occurs when people attribute human behavior to whichever causal  factor  is  most  available to  them. Behavior often “engulfs the field,” and people draw dispositional inferences that correspond to the behavior. When a person freely expresses a certain attitude, others assume that the person believes it. The same inference is biased, however, when  observers know  that  a  powerful other asked the person to express that attitude. The bias is most striking when it is the observers themselves who constrain the respondent’s behavior.

The common interpretation  of the correspondence bias is that it constitutes a “fundamental attribution error” (Ross 1977). This interpretation holds that people fail to fully discount the influence of a person’s internal disposition as a cause of behavior. The error interpretation has been influential in social psychology because it implies that people are incapable of understanding the power of the typical social-psychological experiment, which is to demonstrate that subtle changes in a person’s situation can dramatically change behavior.

Upon review, the idea that people fail to appreciate the power of social situations needs to be tempered. The correspondence bias reverses, for example, when people who know a person’s disposition are asked to judge the strength of the situation. They continue to attribute behavior in part to the situation even when the behavior is freely chosen. Hence, the correspondence bias is generic rather than purely dispositional. People attribute behavior firstly to  whichever causal factor  they  happen  to  be focused on, be it a property of the person or the situation, and  then  modulate  this  inference by  considering the other, less salient causal factor. Because the former process is likely intuitive and  automatic,  whereas the  latter is deliberate and controlled, the bias is larger when people are unmotivated or unable (e.g., because of distraction) to process all available information.

Most models of causal attribution  are hydraulic in that they regard the total causal force directing behavior as a zero-sum quantity. As one causal factor is being favored, another one must yield. On this view, the correspondence bias reflects a failure to fully discount the primary and salient cause when the secondary cause is sufficient. For the explanation of everyday behavior, the hydraulic model is  sometimes  inadequate.  For  example, people  often attribute aggressive behavior to an aggressive disposition. To do so, however, they require the presence of a facilitating stimulus, such as an insult or a threat. Whereas a hydraulic model suggests that inferences about an aggressive disposition  should  be  stronger in  the  absence of provocation, an interactionist model recognizes that a situational cause (provocation) is necessary for a dispositional attribution.  On  this view, theories of personality that  seek to  capture  individual differences by  merely counting trait-related acts are likely contaminated by the researchers’ correspondence biases.

The common tendency of attributing correspondence bias to people’s dispositional failure to think logically may itself be an example of the  very same bias. Correspondence  biases are,  after  all,  experimentally evoked when investigators limit the salience of the situational causes of behavior. Hence, it may be sufficient to attribute respondents’ preference for dispositional inferences to the nature of the experimental situation.

Bibliography:

  1. Gawronski, Ber 2004. Theory-Based Correction in Dispositional Inference: The Fundamental Attribution Error Is Dead, Long Live the Correspondence Bias. In European Review of Social Psychology, vol. 15, ed. Wolfgang Stroebe and Miles Hewstone. Chichester, U.K.: Wiley.
  2. Jones, Edward, and Victor H 1967. The Attribution of Attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 3: 1–24.
  3. Ross, 1977. The Intuitive Psychologist and His Shortcomings. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 10, ed. L. Berkowitz, 174–221. New York: Academic Press.

See also:

Free research papers are not written to satisfy your specific instructions. You can use our professional writing services to buy a custom research paper on any topic and get your high quality paper at affordable price.

ORDER HIGH QUALITY CUSTOM PAPER


Always on-time

Plagiarism-Free

100% Confidentiality
Special offer! Get discount 10% for the first order. Promo code: cd1a428655