Acculturation Research Paper

This sample Acculturation Research Paper is published for educational and informational purposes only. Free research papers are not written by our writers, they are contributed by users, so we are not responsible for the content of this free sample paper. If you want to buy a high quality research paper on any topic at affordable price please use custom research paper writing services.

Abstract

Acculturation is the process of cultural and psychological change that  takes place as a result  of contact  between cultural  groups  and  their  individual  members.  Such contact and change occur during  colonization,  military invasion, migration, and sojourning (e.g., tourism, international  study, overseas posting). It continues  after initial contact in culturally plural societies, where ethnocultural communities maintain features of their heritage cultures. Adaptation to living in culture contact settings takes place over time. Occasionally it is stressful, but  often  it  results  in  some  form  of accommodation. Acculturation  and  adaptation  are now reasonably well understood,  permitting  the development of policies and programs to promote successful outcomes for all parties.

Outline

  1. Acculturation Concept
  2. Acculturation Contexts
  3. Acculturation Strategies
  4. Acculturative Stress
  5. Adaptation
  6. Applications

1. Acculturation Concept

The initial interest in acculturation  grew out of a concern for the effects of European domination  of colonial and indigenous peoples. Later, it focused on how immigrants (both voluntary and involuntary) changed following their  entry and settlement  into receiving societies. More recently, much of the work has been involved with how ethnocultural groups relate to each other,  and to change, as a result of their attempts to live together in culturally plural societies. Nowadays, all three foci are important  as globalization results in ever larger trading and political relations. Indigenous national populations experience neocolonization;  new waves of immigrants, sojourners, and refugees flow from these economic and political  changes; and  large ethnocultural populations become established in most countries.

Early views about  the  nature  of acculturation  are a useful foundation  for contemporary  discussion. Two formulations  in  particular  have been  widely quoted. The  first,  from  Redfield  and  colleagues  in  a  1936 research paper, is as follows:

Acculturation comprehends those phenomena which result when groups of individuals having different cultures   come  into   continuous   first-hand   contact, with subsequent  changes in the original culture  patterns of either or both groups. .. . Under this definition, acculturation  is to be distinguished  from culture change, of which it is but one aspect, and assimilation, which is at times a phase of acculturation.

In another formulation,  the Social Science Research Council in 1954 defined acculturation  as

culture  change  that  is  initiated  by  the  conjunction of two or more autonomous  cultural systems. Acculturative change may be the consequence of direct cultural transmission; it may be derived from non-cultural  causes, such as ecological or demographic  modification induced  by an impinging  culture;  it may be delayed, as with internal  adjustments  following upon the acceptance of alien traits or patterns; or it may be a reactive adaptation of traditional modes of life.

In the first formulation,  acculturation  is seen as one aspect of the broader  concept of culture  change (that which results from intercultural  contact), is considered to generate change in ‘‘either or both groups,’’ and is distinguished   from  assimilation  (which  may  be  ‘‘at times a phase’’). These are important  distinctions  for psychological work and are pursued  later in this research paper. In the second definition,  a few extra features are added, including  change that is indirect  (not  cultural but  rather  ‘‘ecological’’),  is delayed  (internal  adjustments, presumably of both a cultural  and a psychological character, take time), and can be ‘‘reactive’’ (i.e., rejecting the cultural influence and changing toward a more  ‘‘traditional’’  way of life rather  than  inevitably toward greater similarity with the dominant  culture).

In 1967, Graves introduced  the concept of psychological acculturation,   which  refers  to  changes  in  an individual  who  is a participant  in  a culture  contact situation, being influenced both by the external culture and by the changing culture of which the individual is a member. There are two reasons for keeping these two levels distinct.  The first is that  in cross-cultural  psychology, we view individual human behavior as interacting with the cultural context within which it occurs; hence, separate conceptions and measurements are required  at  the  two  levels.  The  second  is  that  not every individual enters into, and participates in, a culture in the same way, nor does every individual change in the same way; there are vast individual differences in psychological acculturation,  even among individuals who live in the same acculturative arena.

A framework  that  outlines  and  links  cultural  and psychological acculturation  and identifies the two (or more)  groups  in contact  is presented  in Fig. 1. This framework serves as a map of those phenomena  that the author believes need to be conceptualized and measured  during  acculturation  research.  At the  cultural level (on the left of the figure), we need to understand  key features  of the  two  original  cultural groups  (A and  B) prior  to  their  major  contact,  the nature of their contact relationships,  and the resulting dynamic cultural  changes in both  groups,  and in the emergent  ethnocultural  groups,  during  the  process of acculturation.  The gathering of this information requires  extensive ethnographic,  community-level work. These changes can be minor or substantial  and can range from being easily accomplished  to being a source of major cultural  disruption.  At the individual level (on the right of the figure), we need to consider the psychological changes that individuals in all groups undergo and their eventual adaptation to their new situations. Identifying these changes requires sampling a population and studying individuals who are variably involved in the process of acculturation.  These changes can be a set of rather  easily accomplished  behavioral shifts (e.g., in ways of speaking,  dressing,  or eating; in one’s cultural identity), or they can be more problematic, producing  acculturative  stress as manifested  by uncertainty,  anxiety, and depression.  Adaptations can be primarily  internal  or psychological (e.g., sense of well-being or self-esteem) or sociocultural,  linking the individual  to others in the new society as manifested, for example, in competence  in the  activities of daily intercultural  living.

             Acculturation Research Paper f1FIGURE 1    A general framework for understanding  acculturation.

2. Acculturation Contexts

As for all cross-cultural  psychology,  it is imperative that we base our work on acculturation  by examining its cultural contexts. We need to understand,  in ethnographic terms, both cultures  that are in contact if we are to understand  the individuals that are in contact.

In  Fig.  1,  we  saw  that  there  are  five aspects  of cultural  contexts: the two original cultures (A and B), the two changing ethnocultural groups (A and B), and the nature of their contact and interactions.

Taking the  immigration  process  as an example,  we may refer  to  the  society of origin  (A), the  society of settlement  (B), and  their  respective  changing  cultural features  following  contact  (A1  and  B1). A complete understanding of acculturation  would need to start with a fairly comprehensive  examination of the societal contexts. In the society of origin, the cultural characteristics that accompany individuals into the acculturation  process need description,  in part  to understand  (literally) where the person is coming from and in part to establish cultural features for comparison with the society of settlement as a basis for estimating an important factor to be discussed  later:  cultural  distance.  The combination  of political, economic, and demographic  conditions  being faced by individuals in their society of origin also needs to be studied as a basis for understanding the degree of voluntariness  in the migration motivation of acculturating individuals.  Arguments by Richmond in 1993 suggested that  migrants  can be arranged  on a continuum between reactive and proactive,  with the former being motivated by factors that are constraining  or exclusionary (and generally negative in character),  and the latter being  motivated   by  factors  that   are  facilitating  or enabling (and generally positive in character). These contrasting   factors  were  also  referred   to   as  push/ pull factors in the earlier literature on migration motivation.

In the society of settlement, a number of factors have importance.  First,  there  are  the  general  orientations that a society and its citizens have toward immigration and pluralism. Some societies have been built by immigration  over  the  centuries,  and  this  process  may be a continuing  one, guided by a deliberate immigration policy. The important  issue to understand  for the process of acculturation  is both the historical  and attitudinal  situations  faced  by migrants  in  the  society  of settlement.  Some societies  are  accepting  of cultural pluralism  resulting  from immigration,  taking steps to support   the  continuation   of  cultural   diversity  as  a shared communal resource. This position represents a positive multicultural  ideology and corresponds to the integration  strategy. Other  societies seek to eliminate diversity  through  policies  and  programs  of assimilation, whereas still others attempt to segregate or marginalize diverse populations  in their societies. Murphy argued in 1965 that societies that are supportive of cultural  pluralism  (i.e.,  with  a positive  multicultural ideology) provide a more positive settlement  context for two reasons.  First, they are less likely to enforce cultural  change  (assimilation)  or exclusion  (segregation and marginalization)  on immigrants. Second, they are more likely to provide social support both from the institutions  of the larger society (e.g., culturally sensitive health care, multicultural curricula in schools) and from the continuing  and evolving ethnocultural communities that usually make up pluralistic societies. However, even where pluralism  is accepted, there are well-known variations in the relative acceptance of specific cultural,  ‘‘racial,’’ and religious groups. Those groups that are less well accepted experience hostility, rejection,  and  discrimination,  one  factor that  is predictive of poor long-term adaptation.

3. Acculturation Strategies

Not all groups and individuals undergo acculturation  in the same way; there are large variations in how people seek to engage the process. These variations have been termed  acculturation   strategies.  Which  strategies  are used depends  on a variety of antecedent  factors (both cultural and psychological), and there are variable consequences  (again  both  cultural  and  psychological)  of these  different  strategies.  These  strategies  consist  of two (usually related) components: attitudes and behaviors (i.e., the preferences and actual outcomes) that are exhibited in day-to-day intercultural  encounters.

The centrality of the concept of acculturation  strategies can be illustrated by reference to each of the components  included  in Fig. 1. At the cultural  level, the two groups in contact (whether  dominant  or nondominant)  usually have some notion about what they are attempting  to do (e.g., colonial  policies,  motivations for migration), or of what is being done to them, during  the contact.  Similarly, the elements of culture that will change depend  on the group’s acculturation strategies.  At the  individual  level, both  the  behavior changes and acculturative  stress phenomena  are now known  to  be a function,  at  least to  some  extent,  of what people try to do during  their acculturation,  and the longer term outcomes (both psychological and sociocultural adaptations) often correspond to the strategic  goals  set  by  the  groups   of  which   they  are members.

Four   acculturation   strategies  have  been  derived from two basic issues facing all acculturating  peoples. These issues are based on the distinction between orientations  toward one’s own group and those toward other groups. This distinction  is rendered  as a relative preference  for maintaining  one’s heritage culture  and identity and as a relative preference for having contact with and participating  in the larger society along with other  ethnocultural groups.  This formulation  is presented in Fig. 2.

These two issues can be responded to on attitudinal dimensions,  represented  by bipolar  arrows.  For purposes  of presentation,  generally  positive  or  negative orientations to these issues intersect to define four acculturation strategies. These strategies carry different names, depending  on which ethnocultural group (the dominant  or nondominant one)  is being considered. From the point of view of nondominant groups (on the left of Fig. 2), when individuals do not wish to maintain their cultural identity and instead seek daily interaction with other cultures,  the assimilation strategy is defined. In contrast, when individuals place a value on holding  onto  their  original  culture  while wishing  to avoid interaction  with others,  the separation  alternative is defined.  When  individuals  have an interest  in maintaining  their original culture while in daily interactions with other groups, integration is the option. In this case, they maintain some degree of cultural integrity  while  seeking,  as  members  of an  ethnocultural group,  to participate  as an integral part  of the larger social network.  Finally,  when  individuals  have little possibility  of,  or  interest   in,  cultural   maintenance (often for reasons of enforced cultural  loss) and little interest in having relations with others (often for reasons of exclusion or discrimination), marginalization is defined.

This presentation  was based on the assumption  that nondominant  groups  and  their  individual   members have the freedom to choose how they want to acculturate. This, of course, is not always the case. When the dominant group enforces certain forms of acculturation or  constrains  the  choices  of nondominant  groups  or individuals, other terms need to be used.

Acculturation Research Paper f2FIGURE 2    Four acculturation  strategies based on two issues in ethnocultural groups and the larger society.

Integration  can be ‘‘freely’’ chosen  and  successfully pursued   by   nondominant   groups   only   when   the dominant society is open and inclusive in its orientation toward cultural  diversity. Thus, a mutual accommodation  is  required   for  integration   to  be  attained  and involves the acceptance by both groups of the right of all groups  to live as culturally  different peoples.  This strategy requires nondominant groups to adopt the basic values of the larger society, whereas the dominant group must  be prepared  to adapt  national  institutions  (e.g., education, health, labor) to better meet the needs of all groups now living together in the plural society.

These two basic issues were initially approached from the  point  of view of the  nondominant ethnocultural groups.  However, the  original  anthropological  definition  clearly  established  that  both  groups  in  contact would become acculturated.  Hence, a third  dimension was added: the powerful role played by the dominant group  in influencing  the way in which mutual  acculturation would take place. The addition of this third dimension produces the right side of Fig. 2. Assimilation, when  sought  by the  dominant  group,  is termed  the ‘‘melting pot’’; however, when it is demanded  by the dominant  group,  it  is  called  the  ‘‘pressure cooker.’’ When separation  is forced by the dominant  group,  it is ‘‘segregation.’’ Marginalization, when imposed by the dominant  group,  is termed  ‘‘exclusion.’’ Finally, integration, when diversity is a feature of the society as a whole (including all of the various ethnocultural groups),  is called ‘‘multiculturalism.’’ With the use of this framework, comparisons can be made between individuals  and  their  groups  and  between  nondominant peoples and the larger society within which they are acculturating.  The ideologies and  policies  of the dominant  group  constitute  an  important  element  of ethnic   relations   research,   whereas   the   preferences of nondominant peoples  are a core feature  in acculturation  research. Inconsistencies and conflicts among these various acculturation  preferences are sources of difficulty  for  acculturating   individuals.   In  general, when acculturation experiences cause problems for acculturating individuals, we observe the phenomenon of acculturative stress.

4. Acculturative Stress

Three  ways in  which  to  conceptualize  outcomes  of acculturation  have been proposed  in the literature.  In the first (behavioral shifts), we observe those changes in an individual’s behavioral repertoire that take place rather easily and are usually nonproblematic.  This process  encompasses  three  subprocesses:  culture  shedding; culture  learning;  and culture  conflict. The first two involve the selective, accidental, or deliberate loss of behaviors and their  replacement  by behaviors that allow the individual  a better  ‘‘fit’’  with the society of settlement.  Most often, this process has been termed ‘‘adjustment’’ because  virtually  all  of  the   adaptive changes  take  place  in  the  acculturating   individual, with  few changes  occurring  among  members  of the larger society. These adjustments  are typically made with minimal difficulty, in keeping with the appraisal of the  acculturation   experiences  as  nonproblematic. However, some degree of conflict may occur, and this is usually resolved by the acculturating person yielding to the behavioral norms of the dominant group. In this case, assimilation is the most likely outcome.

When greater levels of conflict are experienced  and the experiences are judged to be problematic but controllable and surmountable, the second approach (acculturative  stress) is the appropriate  conceptualization. In this case, individuals understand  that they are facing problems  resulting  from  intercultural   contact that cannot  be dealt with easily or quickly by simply adjusting or assimilating to them. Drawing on the broader stress and adaptation paradigms, this approach advocates the study of the process of how individuals deal with acculturative problems on first encountering them and over time. In this sense, acculturative stress is a stress reaction  in response  to life events that  are rooted in the experience of acculturation.

A third  approach  (psychopathology)  has had  long use in clinical psychology and psychiatry. In this view, acculturation   is  nearly  always  seen  as  problematic; individuals usually require assistance to deal with virtually insurmountable stressors in their lives. However, contemporary  evidence  shows that  most  people  deal with  stressors  and  reestablish  their  lives rather  well, with  health,  psychological, and  social outcomes  that approximate  those of individuals in the larger society. Instead of using the term ‘‘culture shock’’ to encompass these three approaches, the author prefers to use the term ‘‘acculturative stress’’ for two reasons. First, the notion of shock carries only negative connotations, whereas stress can vary from positive (eustress) to negative (dis-stress) in valence. Because acculturation has both positive (e.g., new opportunities) and negative (e.g., discrimination) aspects, the stress conceptualization  better matches the range of affect experienced during acculturation. Moreover, shock has no cultural or psychological theory or research context associated with it, whereas stress (as noted previously) has a place in a well-developed theoretical matrix  (i.e., stress–coping–adaptation). Second, the phenomena of interest have their life in the intersection of two cultures; they are intercultural,  rather than cultural, in their origin. The term ‘‘culture’’ implies that only one culture  is involved, whereas the term ‘‘acculturation’’ draws our attention to the fact that two cultures are interacting  with,  and  producing,  the  phenomena. Hence, for both reasons, the author  prefers the notion of acculturative stress to that of culture shock.

Relating  these  three   approaches   to  acculturation strategies, some consistent empirical findings allow the following generalizations. For behavioral shifts, the fewest behavioral changes result from the separation strategy, whereas most result from the assimilation strategy. Integration involves the selective adoption of new behaviors  from  the  larger  society  and  retention  of valued features  of one’s  heritage  culture.  Marginalization  is often associated  with  major  heritage  culture  loss and the appearance of a number of dysfunctional and deviant behaviors (e.g., delinquency, substance abuse, familial  abuse).  For  acculturative  stress,  there  is  a  clear picture  that the pursuit  of integration  is least stressful (at least where it is accommodated  by the larger society), whereas marginalization  is the most stressful. In between are the assimilation and separation  strategies, with sometimes one and sometimes the other being the less stressful. This pattern of findings holds for various indicators of mental health.

5. Adaptation

As a result of attempts to cope with these acculturation changes, some long-term adaptations  may be achieved. As mentioned earlier, adaptation refers to the relatively stable changes that take place in an individual or group in response to external demands. Moreover, adaptation may or may not improve the fit between individuals and their environments.  Thus, it is not a term that necessarily implies that individuals or groups change to become more like their environments (i.e., adjustment by way of assimilation), but it may involve resistance and attempts to change  their  environments  or to move away from them altogether (i.e., by separation). In this use, adaptation is an outcome that may or may not be positive in valence (i.e., meaning only well adapted).  This bipolar sense of the concept of adaptation is used in the framework in Fig. 1 where long-term adaptation to acculturation  is highly variable, ranging  from well adapted  to poorly adapted and varying from a situation where individuals can manage their new lives very well to one where they are unable to carry on in the new society.

Adaptation is also multifaceted. The initial distinction between psychological and sociocultural adaptation was proposed and validated by Ward in 1996. Psychological adaptation largely involves one’s psychological and physical well-being, whereas sociocultural adaptation refers to how well an acculturating  individual  is able to  manage  daily  life  in  the  new  cultural   context. Although  conceptually  distinct,  the  two measures  are empirically related to some extent (correlations between them are in the +.40 to +.50 range). However, they are also empirically distinct  in the sense that they usually have different  time  courses  and  different  experiential predictors.  Psychological problems often increase soon after contact, followed by a general (but variable) decrease over time. However, sociocultural  adaptation typically has a linear improvement  with time. Analyses of the  factors  affecting adaptation  reveal  a  generally consistent pattern. Good psychological adaptation is predicted  by personality  variables, life change events, and social support,  whereas good sociocultural  adaptation is predicted by cultural knowledge, degree of contact, and positive intergroup  attitudes.

Research relating adaptation  to acculturation  strategies  allows   for   some   further   generalizations.   For all three  forms of adaptation,  those  who  pursue  and accomplish integration appear to be better adapted, whereas those who are marginalized are least well adapted. And again, the assimilation and separation strategies are associated with intermediate adaptation outcomes.   Although  there   are  occasional  variations on this pattern, it is remarkably consistent and parallels the generalization regarding acculturative stress.

6. Applications

There is now widespread evidence that most people who have experienced acculturation actually do survive. They are not destroyed or substantially diminished by it; rather, they find opportunities and achieve their goals, sometimes beyond their initial imaginings. The tendency to ‘‘pathologize’’ the  acculturation  process and  outcomes may be due partly to the history of its study in psychiatry and  in  clinical  psychology. Second, researchers  often presume  to know what acculturating  individuals  want and impose their own ideologies or personal views rather than informing themselves about culturally rooted individual preferences and differences. One key concept (but certainly not the only one) in understanding  this variability, acculturation  strategies, has been emphasized in this research paper.

The  generalizations  that  have  been  made  in  this research paper on the basis of a wide range of empirical findings allow us to propose that public policies and programs that seek to reduce  acculturative  stress and to improve intercultural  relationships  should  emphasize the integration  approach  to acculturation.  This is equally true of national policies, institutional  arrangements, and the goals of ethnocultural groups. It is also true of individuals in the larger society as well as members of nondominant acculturating  groups.

In some countries, the integrationist  perspective has become legislated in policies of multiculturalism that encourage and support the maintenance of valued features of all cultures while supporting  full participation of all ethnocultural groups in the evolving institutions of the larger society. What seems certain is that cultural  diversity  and  the  resultant  acculturation  are here to stay in all countries. Finding a way in which to accommodate each other poses a challenge and an opportunity  to social and cross-cultural  psychologists everywhere.  Diversity is a fact of contemporary  life. Whether  it is the ‘‘spice of life’’ or the main ‘‘irritant’’ is probably the central question that confronts us all—citizens and social scientists alike.

References:

  1. Berry,   W.  (1976).   Human  ecology and  cognitive style: Comparative  studies  in  cultural  and  psychological adaptation. New York: Russell Sage/Halsted.
  2. Berry, J. W. (1980). Acculturation as varieties of adaptation. In A. Padilla (Ed.), Acculturation: Theory, models, and findings (pp. 9–25). Boulder, CO: Westview.
  3. Berry, J. W. (1997). Immigration, acculturation, and adaptation. Applied Psychology, 46, 5–68.
  4. Berry, J. W., Kalin, R., & Taylor, D. (1977). Multiculturalism and ethnic attitudes in Canada. Ottawa, Ontario: Supply & Services.
  5. Berry, J. W., & Kim, U. (1988). Acculturation  and mental health.  In P. Dasen, J. W. Berry, &  Sartorius (Eds.), Health and  cross-cultural psychology (pp.  207–236). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
  6. Berry, J. W., Kim, U., Minde, T., & Mok, D. (1987). Comparative studies  of acculturative  International Migration Review, 21, 491–511.
  7. Berry, J. W., Kim, U., Power, S., Young, M., & Bujaki, M. (1989). Acculturation attitudes in plural societies. Applied Psychology, 38, 185–206.
  8. Berry, J. W., Poortinga, Y. H., Segall, M. H., & Dasen, P. R. (2002). Cross-cultural  psychology: Research and  applications (2nd ed.). New York: Cambridge University Press.
  9. Graves, T. (1967). Psychological acculturation in a tri-ethnic community.   South-Western Journal  of Anthropology, 23, 337–350.
  10. Murphy, H. B. M. (1965). Migration and the major mental disorders. In M. B. Kantor (Ed.), Mobility and mental health (pp. 221–249). Springfield, IL: Thomas.
  11. Ogbu, U. (1992).  Understanding  cultural  diversity  and learning. Educational Researcher, 21, 5–14.
  12. Redfield, ,   Linton,    R.,   &    Herskovits,    M.   (1936). Memorandum   on  the  study  of acculturation.   American Anthropologist, 38, 149–152.
  13. Richmond, (1993).  Reactive migration:  Sociological perspectives on refugee movements. Journal of Refugee Studies, 6, 7–24.
  14. Social Science Research Council. (1954).  Acculturation:  An exploratory      American  Anthropologist, 56, 973–1002.
  15. Ward, C. (1996).   In D. Landis, & R. Bhagat (Eds.),   Handbook  of  intercultural   training   (2nd   ed., pp. 124–147). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
  16. Ward, C., Bochner, S., & Funham, A. (2001). The psychology of culture shock. London: Routledge.

See also:

Free research papers are not written to satisfy your specific instructions. You can use our professional writing services to order a custom research paper on any topic and get your high quality paper at affordable price.

ORDER HIGH QUALITY CUSTOM PAPER


Always on-time

Plagiarism-Free

100% Confidentiality
Special offer! Get discount 10% for the first order. Promo code: cd1a428655